home *** CD-ROM | disk | FTP | other *** search
- March 1991
-
-
- COGNITIVE INTERVIEWING
-
- By
-
- Margo Bennett, M.Ed.
- and
- John E. Hess, M.Ed.
- Special Agents and Instructors
- FBI Academy
- Quantico, Virginia
-
-
- When interviewing crime victims, few investigators begin
- with questions such as: How tall was the subject? What color
- was his hair? Did he have any scars? Common sense, experience,
- and fundamental training lead investigators to the conclusion
- that such specific questions give witnesses little opportunity
- to tell what they know. Instead, open-ended questions tend to
- produce the best results. A question like, "What did he look
- like?" eliminates the need for investigators to anticipate every
- detail of description victims may have noted. Investigators can
- always follow up the witness' statements with specific, direct
- questions to fill in gaps. At least, that is what many
- interview textbooks suggest. But what happens when even these
- direct questions fail to produce the details needed from
- witnesses? The cognitive interview method is a proven
- technique, effective because it provides interviewers with a
- structured approach to help retrieve such details from the
- memories of witnesses.
-
- Consider the following scenario: At a robbery scene, a
- uniformed officer briefs the investigating detective. Hoping to
- obtain additional information, the detective approaches the
- clerk, introduces himself, and sensing her anxiety, takes some
- time to assure her that she has nothing to worry about. He
- tells her he understands the trauma she has just undergone, gets
- her a cup of coffee, and delays asking any questions until she
- has regained her composure. He then tells her that he needs her
- help and asks that she start at the beginning and tell him
- exactly what happened. She replies:
-
- "I was behind the counter when all of a sudden, I heard a
- voice telling me to give him all the money, and I would not
- get hurt. I looked up and saw a man wearing a ski mask
- pointing a gun right at me. I just froze and stared at the
- gun. He told me to get a move on or there would be trouble.
- I opened the cash register and handed him all of the bills.
- There was just under a hundred dollars in the register. He
- then told me to lie on the floor and not move. I did as he
- told me and waited until I was sure he was gone. I yelled to
- Joe, the manager, who was in the office, who asked me if I
- was okay. He then ran to the phone and called the police.
- The next thing I knew, the police officer arrived, and I told
- him the same thing I just told you. I don't know what the
- guy looked like, where he came from, or how he got away. I'm
- sorry I can't be more help."
-
- The detective tells her that she has been very helpful and
- that now he would like to go over the story again, and this
- time, if she doesn't mind, he will interrupt her with questions
- as she goes along. As she retells her story, he constantly
- probes for additional details, such as the possibility of
- additional witnesses, more descriptive data regarding the
- subject and his weapon, words he may have used, noticeable
- accent, and the means of his escape. However, except for a bit
- more descriptive data, the victim was correct; she had told the
- responding officer everything she could remember.
-
- THE PROBLEM: INABILITY TO REMEMBER
-
- The above scenario illustrates a problem encountered by
- many investigators. That problem results not from investigators
- being unable to ask good questions but simply from witnesses who
- are unable to provide the answers. Responses such as, "I don't
- remember," "That's all I saw," or "I can't recall" frustrate
- many interviewers on a regular basis. In the past, this led
- investigators to try hypnosis as a means of enhancing witness
- recall. Improved results verified what many investigators
- suspected--an inability of witnesses to remember, not a lack of
- observations, was the main problem. (1) Although investigators
- achieved some success through hypnosis, those successes did not
- last long. Courts, on a regular basis, began ruling in favor of
- defense attorneys who alleged that hypnotically elicited
- information may contain flaws and that hypnosis as a means of
- refreshing recall lacks scientific acceptance. (2) Therefore,
- investigators now primarily reserve hypnosis for situations
- where the need for lead information supersedes all other
- considerations. They know full well that using hypnosis will
- probably disqualify a witness from testifying.
-
- SOLVING THE PROBLEM: THE COGNITIVE INTERVIEW
-
- To enhance witness recall without the stigma attached to
- hypnosis, Ronald P. Fisher and Edward Geiselman, professors at
- Florida International University and UCLA respectively, have
- developed a system they call the cognitive interview. Although
- their process contains few, if any, new ideas, they have
- systematized some techniques which have, for the most part, been
- used by investigators only in a sporadic, piecemeal fashion.
- Research indicates that the cognitive approach to interviewing
- witnesses increases the quantity of information obtained (3) and
- does not jeopardize the witness' credibility in court, as
- hypnosis does.
-
- This article compares the traditional interview with the
- cognitive interview. Specifically, this article deals with the
- cognitive interview technique as it assists witness memory
- retrieval by: 1) Reinstating the context of the event, 2)
- recalling the event in a different sequence, and 3) looking at
- the event from different perspectives. It also deals with
- specific retrieval techniques and time factors that affect the
- interview.
-
- Reinstate the Context
-
- Traditional interviews of victims and witnesses, similar to
- the one described above, usually begin with interviewers first
- taking the time to make introductions and putting witnesses at
- ease before asking, "What happened?" or "What can you tell me
- about...?" Then, specific questions follow that are geared to
- fill in the gaps inadvertently left by witnesses. Proponents of
- the cognitive interview suggest this will not usually produce
- optimum results. Asking people to isolate an event in their
- minds and then to verbalize that event requires them to operate
- in a vacuum. Even without the trauma that often results from
- involvement in a crime, common sense says that human memory
- functions better in context. The cognitive interview process
- takes this into account.
-
- What is meant by context and how do interviewers establish
- it? Simply put, interviewers make efforts to reestablish the
- environment, mood, setting, and experiences by asking witnesses
- to relive mentally the events prior to, during, and after the
- crime.
-
- Let's return to the robbery scene described above with the
- detective who had already introduced himself to the victim and
- asked for her help. Instead of asking her what happened during
- the crime, using the cognitive interview approach, he proceeds
- as follows: "It's only about 10:00, and it's already been a
- pretty full day for you. How about telling me how your day
- started. Tell me what time you got up, the chores you did, the
- errands you ran and anything else that happened before you came
- to work."
-
- As she recounts her activities, he joins the conversation,
- discussing events with her, including the problems of a working
- mother, what she fixed for breakfast, and any other details that
- she mentions. Only when they have developed a clear picture of
- those events does the detective next suggest that the victim
- describe her travel to work. He handles this portion of the
- conversation in the same way. He does not ask perfunctory
- questions geared to getting her quickly to the crime scene, but
- rather, he discusses her commute to work in depth. They discuss
- the route she took, weather and traffic conditions she
- encountered, events she may have noticed, and finally, where she
- parked her car and what she noticed at that time. He wants her
- not only to just describe her day in general but also to relive
- it.
-
- He uses the same interview technique regarding her arrival
- at work. By the time they finally get to the discussion of the
- robbery, they have put the event into context. In many
- instances, this process enhances measurably a person's retrieval
- of stored information. Thus, witnesses can see details of the
- robbery in their proper sequence and context. Concentration is
- more focused than during any previous interviews, which may have
- only consisted of isolated questions and answers. The response,
- "I can't remember," will occur less frequently.
-
- Change Sequence
-
- To continue the interview and further develop the witness'
- recall, another phase of the cognitive interview follows next in
- sequence. Initially, retrieving information from witnesses
- occurs in a normal, chronological flow of events. However, when
- recounting from memory, people tend to edit as memory playback
- occurs. This results in a summary based upon what witnesses
- regard as important. Therefore, interviewers should address
- this problem by prompting witnesses not to hold back even the
- most insignificant detail. Even so, most interviewers can cite
- experiences where valuable information went unmentioned because
- witnesses chose to omit it.
-
- By changing the sequence of recall, witnesses can look at
- each stage of the event as a separate entity much akin to
- looking at individual frames from a film. Reverse or
- out-of-order recall also encourages an overly zealous witness to
- stick to the facts. Witnesses find it more difficult to
- embellish the event when they separate themselves from the
- natural flow of events and independently deal with each
- activity.
-
- Returning to the eye-witness interview in the opening
- scenario, the detective might continue using the cognitive
- interview technique. Accordingly, he would discuss the
- conversation the victim had with the responding officer and ask
- where she was when the officer arrived. He wants to know
- exactly what she was doing at that time. What did she do
- immediately before that? Through this line of questioning, he
- gradually arrives back at the time of the robbery and before
- hand. Thus, he leads her through a second recounting of the
- crime, only in reverse sequence. This time, her information is
- a collection of pieces, each viewed independently. Just as
- looking at a portion of the landscape may reveal details missed
- while taking in the panoramic view, looking at stages of an
- event may enable witnesses to "see" previously unnoticed items.
-
- Change Perspective
-
- To further stimulate witness memory recovery, Fisher and
- Geiselman also suggest changing the perspective. (4) Witnesses
- experience an event one time; however, they may perceive it from
- various views. During initial recollection, witnesses
- articulate from their personal perspectives and rarely vary from
- their point of view. By prompting witnesses to physically
- change the positioning in their memories, interviewers give them
- the opportunity to recall more of their experiences. (5)
- Interviewers can change perspective by asking witnesses to
- consider the view of another witness, victim, or an invisible
- eye on the wall.
-
- Using the technique of changing the perspective of
- witnesses, the detective in the opening scenario might say: "You
- know those surveillance cameras they have in banks and some
- stores? Too bad there wasn't one on the wall over there. I
- wonder just what it would have recorded; it certainly would have
- had a different vantage point than you did." Through this
- opening statement, he can draw the victim into a discussion of
- what might have been recorded on the nonexistent camera. This
- technique not only provides her with an opportunity to "replay"
- the event from a different perspective but it also serves to
- further detraumatize the situation. Reviewing a film is much
- less traumatic than reliving an armed robbery.
-
- SPECIFIC RETRIEVAL
-
- Interviewers can use additional techniques to promote
- memory retrieval, depending on the facts of the crime and
- witness information. After witnesses have recounted an event in
- its natural sequence, reverse sequence, and from different
- perspectives, the interviewer can induce specific retrieval by
- asking direct questions. One technique of specific retrieval
- includes associating witness recollection of physical
- appearance, clothing, and sound with something or someone
- familiar to them. Other areas of recall, such as remembering
- names and numbers, may be enhanced by dealing with individual
- components of the item, such as the first letter or number.
- Once established, interviewers direct concentration to the next
- letter or number and build the response.
-
- Using this technique, the detective in the robbery scenario
- might have first reviewed the details obtained thus far. At
- certain points, he might have stopped to ask questions such as:
- "You say he had a scary voice. How so? Does it remind you of
- anybody you know, or perhaps somebody you've seen in a movie?"
- "The coveralls he was wearing--ever seen that type before?
- Where? Were they like a pilot's suit, or more like a
- carpenter's?"
-
- This context-enhancing technique stems from realizing that
- the victim did not experience this event as a clean slate. She
- had a lifetime of experiences that preceded this activity.
- Therefore, when getting a description of the subject, a
- detective's questions, "Does this person remind you of anyone
- you know? In what way?" likewise provide a context from which
- the victim can make comparisons. This removes her need to
- create, thus enabling her to draw on information with which she
- is comfortable.
-
- TIME FACTORS
-
- The cognitive interview encourages a witness' in-depth
- retrieval of memory. Success with this technique, although a
- time-consuming process, forces interviewers to avoid some traps
- normally associated with police interviews, specifically,
- rushing the recall of witnesses and interrupting their
- narratives.
-
- Witnesses must feel confident that they have time to think,
- speak, reflect, and speak again as often as they need.
- Interviewers can instill this confidence by allowing sufficient
- time for the interview and by refraining from interrupting
- witnesses. (6) All too often, interviewers say, "Tell me what
- happened," but before witnesses speak for 30 seconds,
- interviewers begin interrupting with specific questions. Those
- specific questions should be asked after witnesses have had the
- opportunity to recount the event fully. Allowing time to
- respond also applies when witnesses answer specific retrieval
- questions. Rushing witnesses sends a message to them that their
- information is trivial. This results in witness retrieval
- shutdown. If interviewers don't give them the time, witnesses
- cannot concentrate or remember.
-
- The cognitive interview technique not only enhances witness
- recall but also addresses another common problem among
- interviewers--their inability to sustain the interview.
- Interviewers, particularly inexperienced ones, are often reduced
- to saying, "I can't think of anything else to ask. Is there
- anything you're leaving out?" If a witness responds in the
- negative, the interview is over. Using the cognitive technique
- can help interviewers avoid prematurely reaching this point.
- Experience demonstrates that the cognitive interview technique
- allows interviewers to continue discussing events without
- sounding redundant. Indeed, continued conversation in a
- constructive, helpful direction often prompts additional
- information.
-
- CONCLUSION
-
- Despite significant advances in various forensic fields,
- most crimes are solved by information furnished by people. The
- interview remains the foremost investigative tool for gaining
- information.
-
- Although most victims and witnesses try to cooperate, their
- inability to recall vital details can be discouraging, and they
- need help in remembering. This help must come from
- investigators. Merely asking the right questions does not
- suffice; enhancing someone's memory requires active involvement.
- The cognitive approach to interviewing has proven more effective
- than the traditional one by increasing the quality and quantity
- of information obtained from witnesses and victims.
-
-
- Footnotes
-
- (1) John C. Yuille and N. Hope McEwan, "Use of Hypnosis as
- an Aid to Eyewitness Memory," Journal of Applied Psychology,
- 1985, vol. 70, No. 2, p. 389.
-
- (2) Martin T. Orne, David F. Dinges, and Emily C. Orne,
- "The Forensic Use of Hypnosis," National Institute of Justice,
- December 1984, p. 1.
-
- (3) R. Edward Geiselman, Ronald P. Fisher, David P.
- MacKinnon, and Heidi L. Holland, "Eyewitness Memory Enhancement
- in the Police Interview: Cognitive Retrieval Mnemonics Versus
- Hypnosis," Journal of Applied Psychology, 1985, vol. 70, No. 2,
- p. 403.
-
- (4) R. Edward Geiselman, and Michael Nielsen, "Cognitive
- Memory Retrieval Techniques," The Police Chief, March 1986, p.
- 70.
-
- (5) Ibid.
-
- (6) R. Edward Geiselman, Ronald P. Fisher, David S.
- Raymond, Lynn M. Jurkevich, and Monica L. Warhaftig, "Enhancing
- Eyewitness Memory: Refining the Cognitive Interview," Journal
- of Police Science and Administration, December 1987, vol. 15,
- No. 4, p. 292.
-